Skip to main content

The Car That Knew Too Much - Jean-François Bonnefon ****

This slim book is unusual in taking us through the story of a single scientific study - and it's very informative in the way that it does it. The book makes slightly strange reading, as I was one of the participants in the study - but that's not surprising. According to Jean-François Bonnefon, by the time the book was published, around 100 million people worldwide had taken part in the Moral Machine experiment.

The idea behind the study was to see how the public felt self-driving cars should make what are effectively moral decisions. Specifically, in a dilemma where there was a choice to be made between, say, killing one or other person or groups of people, how should the car decide? As a concept, Bonnefon makes it clear this is a descendent of the classic 'trolley' problem where participants are asked to decide, for example, whether or not to switch the points so a tram that is currently going to kill five people will be switched to a track where it will kill one person who wouldn't otherwise have been harmed.

The many variants of the trolley problem are hard enough to deal with, but as Bonnefon makes clear,  there are more complex determinants when dealing with autonomous vehicles. Apart from anything else, we would have to tell the car how to decide between killing its passenger(s) and a pedestrian. Would anyone buy a car if they were conscious that it would take the choice to intentionally sacrifice them where necessary?

Bonnefon and his colleagues decided to go beyond the usual scope of a study like this (which often just asks a few hundred students) to try to address humanity at large with a simple online system that presented each participant with a short set of preferences between, say, killing a jaywalking child or mounting the pavement and killing an elderly person who just happens to be passing.

In part, this is the story of how such a project works, giving rare insights into what researchers do, down to attempting to get into the big name journals and the fun of getting a bad peer review. This is really interesting, though in an attempt to make the story more approachable, Bonnefon does give us too much detail of the characters of his colleagues and where and how they met. Central to the book, though, is what might be done about autonomous vehicles. We are told of a German commission's findings on the subject, which is then contrasted with the findings from the study. (The main disparity is that the commission felt that cars should not be discriminatory, where the public at large thought they should be ageist.)

There are some omissions. When the results were published, one of the criticisms Bonnefon and colleagues got was that they asked people to make choices based on information that autonomous vehicles could never access - for example, choosing between a professional and a homeless person, a distinction the car couldn't possibly make. This seems a bizarre thing to do - surely the study should have been limited to choices that a car could actually make, and Bonnefon does not adequately defend the decision to include these extras.

The book also omits one of the key factors that could influence self-driving car uptake. Bonnefon spends quite some time on the idea that if the cars were only 10 per cent safer than a human driver, then anyone who thought they were at least 10 per cent better than average (which is most people) wouldn't want one. However, he didn't go into the potential problem that if autonomous cars are killing thousands of people, that would be thousands of real families seeing the negative side (and probably suing manufacturers), but the lives saved are statistical, so have nowhere near the same weight. It seems likely that for statistical benefits to outweigh damage caused (as is the case with a vaccine) autonomous vehicles would have to save many more lives than they kill.

There is a very interesting mention of the highly negative reaction received by a car manufacturer's representative, who pointed out that it may be better to definitely save a passenger when weighed against just a possibility of saving a pedestrian, but again Bonnefon doesn't adequately address this very reasonable view. The same issue comes up with many of the analyses of the trolley problem I've seen, where participants will usually switch a trolley to a different track with a remote switch, killing an individual, but won't push an individual off a bridge to their death if it would stop the trolley. This is put down to the remoteness of throwing the switch, but what hardly ever gets mentioned is that there can be a lot more certainty about the effect of throwing the switch compared with pushing someone off a bridge and hoping they somehow stop the trolley.

I would, then, have liked to have seen more detail here - but it's an enjoyable and easy read that both opens a window on what it's like to run such a study and gets the reader thinking about this very real decision that would have to be made if autonomous vehicles are to be allowed free access to our roads (something I suspect won't happen for a long time to come).

Hardback: 
Bookshop.org

  

Kindle 
Using these links earns us commission at no cost to you
Review by Brian Clegg

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Everything is Predictable - Tom Chivers *****

There's a stereotype of computer users: Mac users are creative and cool, while PC users are businesslike and unimaginative. Less well-known is that the world of statistics has an equivalent division. Bayesians are the Mac users of the stats world, where frequentists are the PC people. This book sets out to show why Bayesians are not just cool, but also mostly right. Tom Chivers does an excellent job of giving us some historical background, then dives into two key aspects of the use of statistics. These are in science, where the standard approach is frequentist and Bayes only creeps into a few specific applications, such as the accuracy of medical tests, and in decision theory where Bayes is dominant. If this all sounds very dry and unexciting, it's quite the reverse. I admit, I love probability and statistics, and I am something of a closet Bayesian*), but Chivers' light and entertaining style means that what could have been the mathematical equivalent of debating angels on...

Brainjacking - Brian Clegg *****

Brian Clegg's Brainjacking is a captivating exploration of the subtle and pervasive ways our minds are manipulated in today’s world, offering readers a blend of scientific insight, cultural commentary, and even speculative fiction. Clegg deftly tackles a wide range of topics, from artificial intelligence and deepfakes to psychological nudges and subliminal messaging, weaving them into an engaging narrative that prompts us to rethink how much of our thinking is truly our own. One of the book’s standout strengths is Clegg’s ability to make complex and often unsettling topics accessible and engaging. Whether he's discussing the dangers of AI-generated deepfakes or the psychology behind targeted advertising, Clegg brings a clear, conversational tone to subjects that might otherwise feel daunting. His approach to brainjacking  -  the subtle art of influencing thoughts and behaviours  -  bridges scientific facts with everyday examples, showing how we’re often nudged or ma...

The Genetic Book of the Dead: Richard Dawkins ****

When someone came up with the title for this book they were probably thinking deep cultural echoes - I suspect I'm not the only Robert Rankin fan in whom it raised a smile instead, thinking of The Suburban Book of the Dead . That aside, this is a glossy and engaging book showing how physical makeup (phenotype), behaviour and more tell us about the past, with the messenger being (inevitably, this being Richard Dawkins) the genes. Worthy of comment straight away are the illustrations - this is one of the best illustrated science books I've ever come across. Generally illustrations are either an afterthought, or the book is heavily illustrated and the text is really just an accompaniment to the pictures. Here the full colour images tie in directly to the text. They are not asides, but are 'read' with the text by placing them strategically so the picture is directly with the text that refers to it. Many are photographs, though some are effective paintings by Jana Lenzová. T...